Friday, March 29, 2013

The Social Engineering Toolkit's evolution, goals - CSO Online - Security and Risk

The Social Engineering Toolkit's evolution, goals

Dave Kennedy, creator of social-engineer.org's social engineering toolkit, gives an overview of how the program was created, and how it is always changing to keep pace with crime

.

The Social Engineering Toolkit's evolution, goals

3 tips for using the Social Engineering Toolkit

Analytics Blog: Advanced Topics

Get Useful Insights Easier: Automate Cohort Analysis with Analytics & Tableau

Tuesday, March 12, 2013 | 10:44 AM

Labels: ,

The following is a guest post by Shiraz Asif, Analytics Solutions Architect at E-Nor, a Google Analytics Certified Partner.

Cohort analysis provides marketers with visibility into the behavior of a “class” of visitors, typically segmented by an action on a specific date range. There are many applications and businesses that would benefit tremendously from cohort analysis, including the following sample use cases:
  • What traffic channel yields the most valuable customers (not just valuable one time conversions)
  • Customer life time volume based on their first bought item (or category)
  • Methods for gaining and retaining customers and which groups of customers to focus on
  • For content and media sites, understanding frequency, repeat visitors and content consumption after sign up or other key events
  • Repeat Purchase Probability 
If you read E-Nor President and Principal consultant Feras Alhlou’s latest post on cohort analysis in a cross-platform environment, and read until the very end, you saw a note about a follow up post on how to automate cohort reporting from Google Analytics in Tableau. This is what I'll outline in today’s post. Why the emphasis on automation, you might ask? Without automation, we end up spending more time than necessary on exporting/copying/pasting/massaging data which can eat up resources better used analyzing and optimizing. 

In addition to report automation, data visualization is also key. Google Analytics offers amazing visualization, including the recently announced dashboard enhancements, but at times you also want to view the data and trend it or merge with other sources. For this, its best to use tools available in the Google Analytics Application Gallery or a BI platform like Tableau.

With the introduction out of the way, following is a step-by-step guide to automated, cohort analysis with Google Analytics and Tableau:

1. Cohort Data Elements in Google Analytics

If you have your cohort data elements already captured in Google Analytics, then skip this step, otherwise, this post is on setting up cohort data in by Google’s Analytics Advocate Justin Cutroni is a must.

2. Tableau version 8 (Google Analytics connectors)

In order to automate reports, you need to have Tableau version 8, since this is the version that has a Google Analytics connector (works well, although still in beta).

3. Data Import from Google Analytics Into Tableau
  • From the Tableau home screen, select Connect to Data, and then pick the Google Analytics connector. After authenticating to Google Analytics, you'll be prompted to select your Account, Property and Profile, if you have access to more than one.
  • Set up the data import to get your Custom Variable key (e.g. CV1) and Date as dimensions, and Revenue as a Metric.

4. Tableau Cohort Analysis Configuration
  • Change the format from Google's 20130113 to a Tableau DATE format. Since the date was stored in a custom variable, it was stored as a string. So that Tableau can treat this as a date, we need to convert the string to a date format. This was done by creating a new Calculated field in Tableau. We called the field "Cohort Date". The formula below worked for our purposes but would require some tweaking for larger datasets.
  • Now that we have the date in the format we want, the next step is to subtract the cohort date from the transaction date.  To do this, we created another calculated field called "Days since Signup". The formula for this field was simply:
DATEDIFF('day',[Cohort Date],[Date]). 

Important:  Tableau natively treated this as a "Measure" since it is a number. However since we're going to be graphing this on the X Axis, you should drag it to the Dimensions pane.
  • Drag the Revenue measure to the rows Rows tab. Now drag the Days since Signup to the Columns tab. You should see a long graph similar to:
  • Drag the Cohort date to the Filter pane, and select the cohort dates you'd like to visualize. For ease of use, I suggest, select only a few to begin with. Drag the Cohort to the color shelf to enable color coding of individual cohort dates.
  • Now let's make a couple of adjustments to make the visualization more useful. In the color shelf, click the down arrow next to Cohort Date, and change the default display from Continuous to Discrete. Then, in the same field, select Exact Date instead of Year.
Voila! Your final view should look like this: 

There you have it. With a few steps, we’ve pulled data from Google Analytics via the API using Tableau, massaged the data and then created a very insightful visualization. With this work now done, the graphic can be easily updated/refreshed. This takes the manual and mundane work of setting up the graphic and automates it so we can spend more time analyzing the data and finding hidden insights for our clients.  

Posted by Shiraz Asif, Analytics Solutions Architect at  E-Nor, Google Analytics Certified Partner. Learn more about E-Nor on their website, Google+ or check out their Marketing Optimization blog.

Thursday, March 28, 2013

RekindleIT Gets an Update! « The-Singleton – Dropping Technology Like it's Hot.

RekindleIT Gets an Update!

I've very happy to announce today that RekindleIT is getting some much needed love.

Since it's launch more than a year ago, RekindleIT has steadily grown into being one of the most popular Kindle document solutions on the web. We've been written up just about everywhere and lots of you have written to me to tell me how much RekindleIT means to you.

And I thank you. You are all so totally awesome.

That said, things haven't always been running smoothly. As you might be aware, I work full time as an Engineer and Interaction Designer so finding time to update RekindleIT is tough. After logging into my Analytics account to see how RekindleIT was doing, one thing was clear -- even in spite of problems of undelivered documents, outages, and so on, RekindleIT continues to grow.

So something had to be done.

The areas that needed the most love as I saw it were in two areas:

  • Reliability of document delivery
  • Formatting goodness

For the first problem, I've gone in and basically totally refactored the way RekindleIT operates. It should be much more fault tolerant, and documents will always eventually get delivered (unless there is something wrong with the page). The actual sever process uses far less resources and is more tolerant to problems when it encounters them. If you guys are interesting in hearing the gory details, give me a shout and I'll be more than happy to bludgeon you with more info.

As for formatting, you may notice the lack of "reformat for kindle" option. Well, I've gone ahead and completely refactored the document conversion pipeline and run tests against more that 10,000 documents -- actual documents you guys convert. I think the results are great. I hope you do too.

In the coming weeks I'm going to be making some updates to the actual RekindleIT website too -- at peek times it gets a little overwhelmed, so I'm going to be upgrading the hardware. Stay tuned and again, from the bottom of my heart, thanks for using RekindleIT!

Wednesday, March 27, 2013

Skype Logs!

P1690

Congress Reviews the Copyright Act in the Digital Age - Businessweek

In the year since online activists and technology companies rallied to kill the Stop Online Piracy Act, members of Congress have been anxiously preparing for the inevitable next showdown between people who make things and people who distribute them online. That may begin today—on Wednesday, March 20—when the U.S. Register of Copyrights, Maria Pallante, is set to testify before a House Judiciary Subcommittee. She will press lawmakers to reform the country’s copyright laws, a huge, couple-times-per-century undertaking that will affect movies, music, books, software, and mobile phones.

“I think it is time for Congress to think about the next great copyright act,” Pallante is expected to say, according to an advance copy of her remarks. Under the existing law, the remarks say, “authors do not have effective protections, good faith businesses do not have clear road maps, courts do not have sufficient direction, and consumers and other private citizens are increasingly frustrated.”

Any serious changes to the law would probably take years, pitting the lobbying power of traditional media conglomerates against technology companies and the online activists they sometimes fund. Consumer groups (some also backed by the tech industry) will press for greater freedom for customers to own what they buy, easing restrictions on moving movies and music among devices, sharing e-books, or time-shifting TV shows. None of these thorny problems existed the last time the U.S. passed a major revision of copyright in 1976—generations ago in terms of media and technology. Even that law took more than two decades to complete.

Pallante wants to make copyright “more forward thinking and flexible than before,” according to her testimony, and many of her ideas seemed aimed at updating the law for the digital age. One big issue is “digital first sale”—whether people have the right to sell used copies of digital files they have purchased, such as iTunes songs and Kindle books. In the analog world, the “first sale doctrine” allows consumers to sell used books or CDs. The law is less clear when it comes to digital products, partly because many downloaded works are legally considered to have been licensed, not sold.

Media companies are more interested in how they can assert the rights they have against online piracy. The current notice-and-takedown procedure, created as part of the 1998 Digital Millennium Copyright Act, requires rights-holders to monitor sites for infringing content—a process that has become Sisyphean as the Internet has grown. Pallante also raises the possibility of creating a small-claims process that could allow independent creators to sue pirates without the expense of a federal lawsuit.

Another question that could return to Congress is the idea of a public performance right for sound recordings, which would make traditional radio stations pay the owners—usually labels—as well as songwriters. This is already the practice in most other countries.

Some of the issues Pallante would like to see Congress take up are ones courts have already struggled with, such as the legality of scanning books that are out of print but still under copyright, when the author cannot easily be found. Congress already tried to address this “orphan works” issue without success in 2006 and 2008.

Pallante’s testimony was scheduled by House Judiciary Committee chairman Bob Goodlatte (R-Va.), who has shown interest in updating the law, according to several media business lobbyists, and who has a platform to help to steer it through Congress. Even basic updates to copyright can take years because lawmakers must navigate through centuries of statutes, court decisions, and international treaties, trying to close unwanted loopholes without creating more. That’s one reason Congress has been so slow to revisit issues that are long overdue. The 33-year old current law, Pallante will tell Congress today, is “showing the strain of its age.”

Hacking Wifi Network Using Backtrack.Hack WiFi Network. Wifi

Wifi or Wireless Fidelity is the name of a popular wireless networking technology that uses radio waves to provide wireless high-speed Internet and network connections (as if you didnt know..),Wifi has become an integral part of our lives today. Wifi is secured using a WPA protocol which intends to secure Wireless LANs like Wired LAN’s by encrypting data over radio waves,however, it has been found that WEP is not as secure as once believed.Now almost anyone can hack into a Wifi network by generating the valid WEP key using Bactrack. Read on to learn how..

Disclaimer: This tutorial is given for educational purposes only and that for any misuse of this information; the blogger cannot be held liable.

GETTING BACKTRACK

BackTrack is a slax based top rated Linux live distribution focused on penetration testing which consists of more than 300 up to date tools along with the ability of customizing scripts, configuring and modding kernels which makes it a true gem and a must have for every security enthusiastic out there. The best part – Its free and you can download it from – Remote Exploit

SETTING UP THE CARD AND THE CONSOLE

Boot up Backtrack on your virtual machine/laptop and open up the command console and type the commands as they are given -

ifconfig

This is the Linux equivalent of ipconfig, you will see the network adaptors in your system. See which one is for Wi-Fi. A few examples are wlan0, wifi0, etc.

airmon-ng

This command will initialize the Wi-Fi network monitoring & will tell you how many networks are in range.

airmon-ng stop [Wi-Fi Card name(without the quotes)]

This command will stop the cards broadcast and reception immediately

macchanger –mac [Desired MAC address] [Wi-Fi card name]

This command will change the current MAC address to any MAC address you desire, so that you don’t get caught later

airmon-ng start [Wi-Fi Card name]

You will see another extra adaptor that is set on monitor mode, use that adaptor for all further purposes in the following commands where – “[Wi-Fi card name]” appears

DUMPING PACKETS

Once you have set up all the parameters, you need to sniff and dump data packets in order to get the key. You can do so by using following commands. On the command console type these commands -

airodump-ng [Wi-Fi card name]

Copy and paste the BSSID in the following command and execute it

airodump-ng –c [Channel Number] –w [Desired Filename for later decryption] --bssid [BSSID] [Wi-Fi Card name]

As you execute the command, you will see a certain number of beacons and data packets that will be stored in the filename you have given. The file will be stored in the root of the system drive (Click on Computer and you will see the file).The file will be present in two formats: *.cap, *.txt.

SPEEDING UP THINGS

However packet dumping is quite a slow process, we need to speed up things to save our time. Open new console after the first data packet has been stored and type the command in the new console and execute it.

airreplay-ng -1 0 –a [BSSID] –h [FAKED MAC ADDRESS] -e [Wi-Fi name (you wish to hack)] [Wi-Fi card name]

As you type this command you will see that the data packets required for breaking the key will increase dramatically thereby saving you a lot of time.

REVEALING WEP KEY

Open another console once you have around 20,000 data packets and type the following command to reveal the WEP key.

aircrack-ng –n 64 –b [BSSID] [Filename without the extension]  

wep-wifi-hackedAs you type this command, you will see that a key will appear in front of you in the given below format:
XX:XX:XX:XX

It is not necessary that the key should have exactly the same digits as shown above so please don’t freak out if you see a 10 digit or 14 digit key. Also if the decryption fails, you can change the bit level of the decryption in the command:

aircrack-ng –n [BIT LEVEL] –b [BSSID] [Filename without extension]

Remember, the bit level should be a number of 2n where n:1,2,3,4…
e.g.

aircrack-ng –n 32 –b [BSSID] [Filename without the extension]
OR
aircrack-ng –n 128 –b [BSSID] [Filename without the extension] etc. etc.

Now just login using the WEP key you got.

Cheers..

Monday, March 25, 2013

18 USC 1028A - Aggravated identity theft - Crimes and Criminal Procedure - US Code

Sec. 1028A. Aggravated identity theft

(a)
Offenses.—
(1)
In general.—
Whoever, during and in relation to any felony violation enumerated in subsection (c), knowingly transfers, possesses, or uses, without lawful authority, a means of identification of another person shall, in addition to the punishment provided for such felony, be sentenced to a term of imprisonment of 2 years.
(2)
Terrorism offense.—
Whoever, during and in relation to any felony violation enumerated in section 2332b (g)(5)(B), knowingly transfers, possesses, or uses, without lawful authority, a means of identification of another person or a false identification document shall, in addition to the punishment provided for such felony, be sentenced to a term of imprisonment of 5 years.
(b)
Consecutive Sentence.—
Notwithstanding any other provision of law—
(1) a court shall not place on probation any person convicted of a violation of this section;
(2) except as provided in paragraph (4), no term of imprisonment imposed on a person under this section shall run concurrently with any other term of imprisonment imposed on the person under any other provision of law, including any term of imprisonment imposed for the felony during which the means of identification was transferred, possessed, or used;
(3) in determining any term of imprisonment to be imposed for the felony during which the means of identification was transferred, possessed, or used, a court shall not in any way reduce the term to be imposed for such crime so as to compensate for, or otherwise take into account, any separate term of imprisonment imposed or to be imposed for a violation of this section; and
(4) a term of imprisonment imposed on a person for a violation of this section may, in the discretion of the court, run concurrently, in whole or in part, only with another term of imprisonment that is imposed by the court at the same time on that person for an additional violation of this section, provided that such discretion shall be exercised in accordance with any applicable guidelines and policy statements issued by the Sentencing Commission pursuant to section 994 of title 28.
(c)
Definition.—
For purposes of this section, the term "felony violation enumerated in subsection (c)" means any offense that is a felony violation of—
(1) section 641 (relating to theft of public money, property, or rewards [1]), section 656 (relating to theft, embezzlement, or misapplication by bank officer or employee), or section 664 (relating to theft from employee benefit plans);

(2) section 911 (relating to false personation of citizenship);
(3) section 922 (a)(6) (relating to false statements in connection with the acquisition of a firearm);
(4) any provision contained in this chapter (relating to fraud and false statements), other than this section or section 1028 (a)(7);
(5) any provision contained in chapter 63 (relating to mail, bank, and wire fraud);
(6) any provision contained in chapter 69 (relating to nationality and citizenship);
(7) any provision contained in chapter 75 (relating to passports and visas);
(8) section 523 of the Gramm-Leach-Bliley Act (15 U.S.C. 6823) (relating to obtaining customer information by false pretenses);
(9) section 243 or 266 of the Immigration and Nationality Act (8 U.S.C. 1253 and 1306) (relating to willfully failing to leave the United States after deportation and creating a counterfeit alien registration card);
(10) any provision contained in chapter 8 of title II of the Immigration and Nationality Act (8 U.S.C. 1321 et seq.) (relating to various immigration offenses); or
(11) section 208, 811, 1107(b), 1128B(a), or 1632 of the Social Security Act (42 U.S.C. 408, 1011, 1307 (b), 1320a–7b (a), and 1383a) (relating to false statements relating to programs under the Act).
[1] So in original. Probably should be "records".

Pocket : Cyberwar PsyOps: e-Activism and Social Media

Cyberwar PsyOps: e-Activism and Social Media


By now there is no question that the role of social media supporting the Egyptian protests has confirmed the value of social media in repressive countries. But how do corporations define rules of use which can keep them out of the crosshairs of groups like Anonymous?

As social media becomes more commonplace within corporate workspaces, CIOs have the tough job of working with HR to provide a safe framework in corporate policies on social media.

What exactly is the difference when both Anonymous, famous for distributed denial of service (DDoS) attacks, and other groups claim to merely be peaceful protestors?

Amnesty International Model for Social Media e-Activist

While in San Francisco this past weekend preparing a story about RSA, I happened to come across a Global Day of Action pro-Egyptian rally sponsored in part by Amnesty International (AI). In fact, Amnesty International has actively supported social media usage for several years.

AI defines online activism simply:

An Amnesty International e-activist is an individual who uses information and communication tools – such as mobile phones, blogs, emails or social networking sites – to act for human rights. He or she may also organize, mobilize and inspire online communities of individuals to take action for human rights.

Amnesty International and other nonprofits have been leveraging social media's benefits as early adopters of the technology.

Of particular interest is how they segment the task specialization with roles – such as events organizer, online ambassador, moderators and greeters – on social networking sites.

This can be contrasted with the use of DDoS toolkits, which have become in fashion after the #AnonOps and Anonymous attacks began in earnest supporting WikiLeaks.

Results: Ideas Trump Violence, Presence Trumps Speech

In most Islamic countries, blogging is highly regulated. If we look at the factors surrounding the Egyptian disconnection from the internet, it didn't result from DDoS attacks, it resulted from the media release of brutality in the streets by authorities against protesters. A great deal of media reports leveraged the 'man in the street' with a smartphone, as well as Twitter feeds dedicated to directing protesters in 'command and control' efforts.

Additionally, two #Feb12 San Francisco protestors in support of Egypt's changes filled me in about stateside e-Activists who worked hard to keep the morale and momentum of the call of regime change rolling.

Hager and Sara, two San Francisco protestors I interviewed, are also both part of the Linkster generation. Linksters can be defined as those people who have been born into the technology and have never known a world without social media.

Where GenX and GenY/Millenials adopted the technology (or are still wrestling with it), Linksters have always collaborated with their peers, always broadcast their emotions and opinions, and see the world as much more connected through the internet. My Linkster e-activists elaborated that their tool of choice in #Egypt was Facebook. Neither did any Twittering. Both were extremely concerned when Egypt's internet connectivity went down.

There will be a video of the protest interview available online within a few days.

Of particular note: both confirmed the role Egyptian women are playing in their country's technology is increasing on a huge scale, even if the Islamic culture does not yet recognize women in other social ways. This confirms the hypothesis written about back in Cyberwar PsyOps: Islamic suffrage and social media:

Simply put, internet technology cloaks the identity of the speaker and enhances the consideration of content over presenter. Even at the lowest levels of simply cooperating in Twitter posts, each woman who assists in these protests through the communication layer effectively frees up another [male] body for the more physical protests occurring in the streets. Because of this assistance, we may never know exactly how many women participate.

Bottom line: There is a major change going on within the Arab and Islamic world right now, and technology companies who realize that women are going to be taking a larger part in online communities will be ahead of the game.

BDA: Mubarek 0, Social Media and Linksters: 1

If we're grading by results in both #Tunisia and #Egypt, the pen indeed is mightier than the sword. The online 'pens' are being wielded by younger and younger people through social media. Women are playing a larger role than ever in Islamic technology companies. All of this is key to understanding global risk mitigation for a CIO.

My mood....

Took meds a few hours late but starting to feel better. I turned pc off until I can call the help desk at Columbia - only 29 mins on lifeline phone and I need to buy DVDs or USB to back up data b4 reformatting. I have an unusual error- it is also a very strange frequency - sounds like analog radio but disrupts my connection. Was so close with web site. Deeply disappointed in myself. I even had a title for first post:

"Searching for truth in a web of lies"

I believe Jude is trying to frame me for computer crimes. Like Gary, but I can assure you I have not hacked anything other than my own email account (and unsuccessfully at that!) She claims I "illegally tampered with tweets" I asked a lawyer and legal assistant at my dad's office if that was even a crime. I was kinda hoping it was so I could sue the rhinestone studded jeans of her liposuctioned tummy tucked manicured asset loving ass! Shit, after seeing how many times she used my credentials and oAUTH I could leave for the UK or NZ tomorrow! Just saw two tweets accusing me of soliciting money like one a jerry's kids in a telethon. No doubt one of Jude's sick followers. Accused me of being of a con artist just looking for handouts and charity. The stranger in my timeline sent one to notify the public at large, and the second was directed at me personally telling me that I should be ashamed of myself for taking charity from people who are actually deserving of things like food, toilet paper, and neon tampons on sale for a buck at the dollar general. (where everything is NOT a dollar) Both were sent on Mew Years. I want to puke. And I want real tampons and dryer sheets. Bus fare and enough money buy a day-planner like i had in college and a pack of rollerball pens. The kind they sell in packs of 5 or seven - uniball rollerball bold colours. I used to use them in college to keep my days and thoughts in order. Red was legal, blue health insurance, green was financial records, yellow useless unless it was one of those highlighters with post it notes built in for marking my favourite quotes! Magenta was my favourite colour to journal with on recycled paper. It bad a special feel to it. Guess it was my OCD tactile compulsion!
Who would want to be such an asshole?

Have I ever solicited donations or cash on twitter ?

I sure hope this website doesn't get me in trouble for putting the truth out there. I don't even know the people who say these horrid things so why does it hurt so much? Actually- that is precisely why. Because they are sheep. They have no clue what it feels like to like in this- with so much potential and so little opportunity. That's my morning dose of TMI. Thank you for allowing me to share a bit of sadness and a bit of myself. I know I won't be Elizabeth Wurtzel or Jonathan Kozol, but maybe some lost kid will wonder across these raw posts and feel a little less lonely for a minute. Love and tears for all the lost. Love and grAtitide to you all for allowing me the opportunity to feel my feelings without judgement or resentment. Just me,

@ElyssaD
January 22, 2011

Elyssa Durant - Nashville, TN | MyLife® || AND ANOTHER?

Elyssa Durant -              Nashville, tennessee

Elyssa Durant, 40

Nashville, TN

Gender
Female
Places Lived
Manhasset, NY,  New York, NY,  Madison, TN,  Nashville, TN,  Old Hickory, TN, 

About Elyssa Durant

Elyssa Durant was born in 1972. Elyssa currently lives in Nashville, Tennessee. Before that, she lived in Manhasset, NY from 2000 to 2007. Before that, she lived in New York, NY from 1995 to 2002.

Sign in for FREE to see important information about Elyssa Durant. Sign up to see what Elyssa Durant is up to, and you can even find out who has been looking at information about you on search engines. You can also find and connect with Elyssa Durant's friends, relatives and classmates. If you sign up today you also get free access to our personal relationship manager where you can manage all your emails and facebook messages in one place!

Don't spend another moment searching. Connect with old acquaintances at MyLife.com, the site that brings your social media account and emails under one roof! You now can connect with Elyssa Durant from Nashville, TN as well as manage all your different email and social media accounts. MyLife.com, the ultimate people finder tool! Join us today and find people in Nashville, TN and create your ultimate social network on MyLife.com. Getting a MyLife account also provides you with the ability to find out who's trying to get in touch with you.

Join Now & View the Full Elyssa Durant Profile

Elyssa Durant - Am Qui, TN | MyLife® -- AND ANOTHER

Elyssa Durant -              Am Qui, tennessee

Elyssa Durant, 40

Am Qui, TN

Gender
Female
Places Lived
Manhasset, NY,  Madison, TN,  Nashville, TN,  Old Hickory, TN, 
College
  • Columbia University, New York, NY.
  • Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
Work
  • Mnps

About Elyssa Durant

Elyssa Durant was born in 1972. Elyssa currently lives in Am Qui, Tennessee. Before that, she lived in Manhasset, NY from 2000 to 2007. Before that, she lived in Madison, TN from 2005 to 2006. After high school, she went to Columbia University in New York, NY. she attended college from 1995 to 2000. She majored in Public Administration and Social Services. Vanderbilt University in Nashville, TN. she attended college from 1996 to 1999. She majored in Multi/Interdisciplinary Studies.

Sign in for FREE to see information about Elyssa Durant. As a member you can contact Elyssa Durant directly, and discover who has been doing research about you on the web! As a registered user you can find out what Elyssa Durant's is up to. If you sign up today you also get free access to our personal relationship manager where you can manage all your emails and facebook messages in one place!

Reach out and touch base with old friends only at MyLife.com, the best source for making people connections on the web! Now you can locate Elyssa Durant from Am Qui, TN in seconds thanks to MyLife.com, the leading people search engine & personal management tool! Build bridges with others in Am Qui, TN, and enjoy a life full of interesting people with the help of MyLife.com. Simply sign up and you can also find out who's trying to get in touch with you.

Join Now & View the Full Elyssa Durant Profile

ANOTHER FSKE-- OMG !!!! Elyssa Durant - Madison, TN | MyLife®

Elyssa Durant -              Madison, tennessee

Elyssa Durant, 37

Madison, TN

Gender
Female

About Elyssa Durant

Elyssa Durant was born in 1976. Elyssa currently lives in Madison, Tennessee.

Sign in for FREE to see information about Elyssa Durant. Membership lets you view relevant information about Elyssa Durant, and find out who has been researching your personal info on the web. You can also find and connect with Elyssa Durant's friends, relatives and classmates. Free registration also gets you access to your MyLife's dashboard where you finally can consolidate your various email and social network accounts all in one place.

Find friends and family members from your past at MyLife.com, where it's easy to find and manage your personal relationships all in one place! Look no further. We found Elyssa Durant from Madison, TN using MyLife, the easiest solution for your people search! Say hello to people in Madison, TN, and meet other people! By getting your MyLife account now you will be able to get in touch with old high school and college friends.

Join Now & View the Full Elyssa Durant Profile

http://www.mylife.com/elyssa_durant

Elyssa Durant - Am Qui, TN | MyLife® FOUR NEW FAKES TODAY

Elyssa Durant -              Am Qui, tennessee

Elyssa Durant, 40

Am Qui, TN

Gender
Female
Places Lived
Manhasset, NY,  Madison, TN,  Nashville, TN,  Old Hickory, TN, 
College
  • Columbia University, New York, NY.
  • Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN.
Work
  • Mnps

About Elyssa Durant

Elyssa Durant was born in 1972. Elyssa currently lives in Am Qui, Tennessee. Before that, she lived in Manhasset, NY from 2000 to 2007. Before that, she lived in Madison, TN from 2005 to 2006. After high school, she went to Columbia University in New York, NY. she attended college from 1995 to 2000. She majored in Public Administration and Social Services. Vanderbilt University in Nashville, TN. she attended college from 1996 to 1999. She majored in Multi/Interdisciplinary Studies.

Elyssa Durant can be researched and contacted easily with your FREE MyLife account. As a member you can contact Elyssa Durant directly, and discover who has been doing research about you on the web! As a registered user you can find out what Elyssa Durant's is up to. If you sign up today you also get free access to our personal relationship manager where you can manage all your emails and facebook messages in one place!

Find people and reconnect with your past friends and family members only with MyLife.com, where we help people make connections and manage all online relationships! We are the single best source for finding anyone in the US. For example, you can now find Elyssa Durant from Am Qui, TN in seconds! MyLife.com is, the website designed to help you with your people search! With over 700 million records you can virtually find anyone. Reach out to people from Am Qui, TN, and create your ultimate social network on MyLife.com. Simply sign up and you can also find singles in your area.

Join Now & View the Full Elyssa Durant Profile

Elyssa Durant - Nashville, TN | MyLife®

Elyssa Durant is on MyLife

Elyssa Durant -              Nashville, tennessee

Elyssa Durant, 40

Nashville, TN

Gender
Female
Places Lived
Manhasset, NY,  New York, NY,  Madison, TN,  Nashville, TN,  Old Hickory, TN, 

About Elyssa Durant

Elyssa Durant was born in 1972. Elyssa currently lives in Nashville, Tennessee. Before that, she lived in Manhasset, NY from 2000 to 2007. Before that, she lived in New York, NY from 1995 to 2002.

Sign in for FREE to see important information about Elyssa Durant. Membership lets you view relevant information about Elyssa Durant, and find out who has been researching your personal info on the web. As a registered user you can find out what Elyssa Durant's is up to. A free MyLife account even gives you access to our dashboard where you can manage all your emails, facebook and twitter accounts in one place.

Get the best out of life and make connections with interesting people at MyLife.com, where it's easy to find and manage your personal relationships all in one place! Stop the search for Elyssa Durant who's currently living in Nashville, TN using MyLife.com your best people search option! Find people that you can't find on other sites. Looking for someone in Nashville, TN? We can help! Join now and never lose your contacts again thanks to MyLife.com. With your MyLife profile you can even get profiles found on other sites removed.

Join Now & View the Full Elyssa Durant Profile

O 1 Login = 1 Felony That's a shit load of felonies now that I discovered he has been logging in to my gmail account, and replacing the documents with altered information. That's pretty fucking major. And stupid too. So easy to trace and was so obvious once I took a look at the "HOSTED" images. Not even sure what you call that, but now anyone can make a fake ID from what he lifted out of my private folders and drop box. Oh well, with so many fakes running around, I am more than happy to spend some time with people who knew me before I was jaded, traded, and shaded. Hasta la bye bye Gnashville!

There is a sweet irony in knowing that my official records are going to expose the issues Tennessee has had for YEARS with lax security. Can anyone say Wackenhut? See y'all later. #oops

Internet Crime Complaint Center (IC3) | File a Complaint

File a Complaint

Prior to filing a complaint with the IC3, please read the following information regarding terms and conditions. Should you have additional questions prior to filing your complaint, view FAQ for more information on inquiries such as:

  • What details will I be asked to include in my complaint?
  • What happens after I file a complaint?
  • How are complaints resolved?
  • Should I retain evidence related to my complaint?

The information I've provided on this form is correct to the best of my knowledge. I understand that providing false information could make me subject to fine, imprisonment, or both. (Title 18, U.S. Code, Section 1001)

The IC3 is co-sponsored by the Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI) and the National White Collar Crime Center (NW3C). Complaints filed via this website are processed and may be referred to federal, state, local or international law enforcement or regulatory agencies for possible investigation. I understand any investigation opened on any complaint I file on this website is initiated at the discretion of the law enforcement and/or regulatory agency receiving the complaint information.

Filing a complaint with the IC3 in no way serves as notification to my credit card company that I am disputing unauthorized charges placed on my card or that my credit card number may have been compromised. I should contact my credit card company directly to notify them of my specific concerns.

Advisory:

You are about to file a complaint with the Internet Crime Complaint Center. The confidentiality of the information you provide may be affected by state law. As such, we cannot guarantee that your complaint will remain confidential. The complaint information you submit to this site is encrypted via secure socket layer (SSL) encryption. Please see the Privacy Policy for further information.

We thank you for your cooperation.

Vulnerability Note VU#625617 - Java 7 fails to restrict access to privileged code

Vulnerability Note VU#625617

Java 7 fails to restrict access to privileged code

Original Release date: 10 Jan 2013 | Last revised: 18 Mar 2013

Overview

Java 7 Update 10 and earlier versions of Java 7 contain a vulnerability that can allow a remote, unauthenticated attacker to execute arbitrary code on a vulnerable system.

Description

The Oracle Java Runtime Environment (JRE) 1.7 allows users to run Java applications in a browser or as standalone programs. Oracle has made the JRE available for multiple operating systems. OpenJDK is an open-source implementation of the Java platform, and the IcedTea project aims to make it easier to deploy OpenJDK, including a web browser plugin.

The Java JRE plug-in provides its own Security Manager. Typically, a web applet runs with a security manager provided by the browser or Java Web Start plugin. Oracle's document states, "If there is a security manager already installed, this method first calls the security manager's checkPermission method with a RuntimePermission("setSecurityManager") permission to ensure it's safe to replace the existing security manager. This may result in throwing a SecurityException".

By leveraging the a vulnerability in the Java Management Extensions (JMX) MBean components, unprivileged Java code can access restricted classes. By using that vulnerability in conjunction with a second vulnerability involving recursive use of the Reflection API via the invokeWithArguments method of the MethodHandle class, an untrusted Java applet can escalate its privileges by calling the the setSecurityManager() function to allow full privileges, without requiring code signing. Oracle Java 7 update 10 and earlier Java 7 versions are affected. OpenJDK 7, and subsequently IcedTea, are also affected. The invokeWithArguments method was introduced with Java 7, so therefore Java 6 is not affected.

This vulnerability is being attacked in the wild, and is reported to be incorporated into exploit kits. Exploit code for this vulnerability is also publicly available. We have confirmed that Oracle Java 7 installed on Windows, OS X, and Linux platforms are affected. Other platforms that use Oracle Java 7 may also be affected.

Impact

By convincing a user to visit a specially crafted HTML document, a remote attacker may be able to execute arbitrary code on a vulnerable system. Note that applications that use the Internet Explorer web content rendering components, such as Microsoft Office or Windows Desktop Search, may also be used as an attack vector for this vulnerability.

Solution

Apply an update

Oracle Security Alert CVE-2013-0422 states that Java 7 Update 11 addresses this (CVE-2013-0422) and an equally severe, but distinct vulnerability (CVE-2012-3174). Immunity has indicated that only the reflection vulnerability has been fixed and that the JMX MBean vulnerability remains. Java 7u11 sets the default Java security settings to "High" so that users will be prompted before running unsigned or self-signed Java applets.

Unless it is absolutely necessary to run Java in web browsers, disable it as described below, even after updating to 7u11. This will help mitigate other Java vulnerabilities that may be discovered in the future.

This issue has also been addressed in IcedTea versions 2.1.4, 2.2.4, and 2.3.4.

Disable Java in web browsers

Starting with Java 7 Update 10, it is possible to disable Java content in web browsers through the Java control panel applet. Please see the Java documentation for more details.

Note: Due to what appears to potentially be a bug in the Java installer, the Java Control Panel applet may be missing on some Windows systems. In such cases, the Java Control Panel applet may be launched by finding and executing javacpl.exe manually. This file is likely to be found in C:\Program Files\Java\jre7\bin or C:\Program Files (x86)\Java\jre7\bin.

Also note that we have encountered situations on Windows where Java will crash if it has been disabled in the web browser as described above and then subsequently re-enabled. Depending on the browser used, this Michael Horowitz has pointed out that performing the same steps on Windows 7 will result in unsigned Java applets executing without prompting in Internet Explorer, despite what the "Security Level" slider in the Java Control panel applet is configured to use. We have confirmed this behavior with Internet Explorer on both Windows 7 and Vista. Reinstalling Java appears to correct both of these situations.

System administrators wishing to deploy Java 7 Update 10 or later with the "Enable Java content in the browser" feature disabled can invoke the Java installer with the WEB_JAVA=0 command-line option. More details are available in the Java documentation.


Restrict access to Java applets

Network administrators unable to disable Java in web browsers may be able to help mitigate this and other Java vulnerabilities by restricting access to Java applets. This may be accomplished by using proxy server rules, for example. Blocking or whitelisting web requests to .jar and .class files can help to prevent Java from being used by untrusted sources. Filtering requests that contain a Java User-Agent header may also be effective. For example, this technique can be used in environments where Java is required on the local intranet. The proxy can be configured to allow Java requests locally, but block them when the destination is a site on the internet.

Integrating intention and context: assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome

Integrating intention and context: assessing social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome

Abstract

Deficits in social cognition are an evident clinical feature of the Asperger syndrome (AS). Although many daily life problems of adults with AS are related to social cognition impairments, few studies have conducted comprehensive research in this area. The current study examined multiple domains of social cognition in adults with AS assessing the executive functions (EF) and exploring the intra and inter-individual variability. Fifteen adult's diagnosed with AS and 15 matched healthy controls completed a battery of social cognition tasks. This battery included measures of emotion recognition, theory of mind (ToM), empathy, moral judgment, social norms knowledge, and self-monitoring behavior in social settings. We controlled for the effect of EF and explored the individual variability. The results indicated that adults with AS had a fundamental deficit in several domains of social cognition. We also found high variability in the social cognition tasks. In these tasks, AS participants obtained mostly subnormal performance. EF did not seem to play a major role in the social cognition impairments. Our results suggest that adults with AS present a pattern of social cognition deficits characterized by the decreased ability to implicitly encode and integrate contextual information in order to access to the social meaning. Nevertheless, when social information is explicitly presented or the situation can be navigated with abstract rules, performance is improved. Our findings have implications for the diagnosis and treatment of individuals with AS as well as for the neurocognitive models of this syndrome.

Keywords: Asperger syndrome, contextual social cognition, executive functions, individual variability

Introduction

Social cognition refers to specific information processing involved in the successful navigation of challenges related to survival and reproduction in social species (Adolphs, 1999). The construct of social cognition involves several domains, including emotional processing, theory of mind (ToM), decision-making, empathy, moral judgment, and social norms knowledge, among others. Despite the seemingly differences in these components, some of them require similar underlying processes. Multiple social cognition domains require the spontaneous perception of the relevant social elements of the situation and the interpretation of how these elements create a given social context (Klin, 2000), which depends on the implicit inference of contextual clues that bias the social meaning of an action (Ibañez and Manes, 2012). For example, emotional recognition of a face usually occurs within a background that includes emotional body language and other convergent information such as prosody, gestures, and situational clues. In contrast, other processes may require the use of explicit and abstract rules about the general social setting in terms of conventions or expected behaviors (e.g., explicit social norms during specific social interactions). Thus, different strategies underlie the different social cognition domains. Here, we investigate different aspects of social cognition in adults with Asperger syndrome (AS).

AS is a pervasive developmental disorder characterized by severe and sustained impairments in social interaction and the development of restricted, repetitive patterns of behavior, interest, and activities. These disturbances must cause significant impairments in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; Matson and Wilkins, 2008). AS may be distinguished from autistic disorder by a lack of delay in early language development (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005). Because the main focus has been on early recognition and diagnosis, this syndrome has primarily been studied in children. However, given that AS is a chronic lifelong condition and nuclear symptoms persist, research in adults has recently received particular attention (Fombonne and Tidmarsh, 2003; Lugnegard et al., 2011).

Recent reports suggest that adults with AS exhibit deficits in multiple social cognition domains including face recognition, emotional processing, ToM, empathy, and moral judgment (see below). Nevertheless, previous studies have not taken into account several factors that should be considered simultaneously in the social cognition research of these individuals. These factors include: (1) the simultaneous assessment of multiple social cognition domains, (2) the sample selection, (3) the assessment of executive functions (EF), and (4) the cognitive heterogeneity of the AS. In the present study, we considered all of these aspects, which are essential for establishing the underlying factors that contribute to the social cognition deficits of adults with AS.

Social cognition disturbances in adults with AS

Emotional processing is an emerging topic of interest. There are numerous reports of individuals with autism spectrum disorders [autism, high functioning autism (HFA), and AS] being impaired in both recognition (Hobson et al., 1988; Ashwin et al., 2006; Hubert et al., 2007; Atkinson, 2009) and production of emotional expressions (Macdonald et al., 1989). Studies focused in adults with AS (Philip et al., 2010) show deficits on emotion recognition from faces [especially negative emotions (Ashwin et al., 2007; Falkmer et al., 2011)]. Thus, evidence suggests that emotional processing is affected in AS and other autism spectrum disorders.

ToM is another area of interest in AS research, since it requires the ability to infer the beliefs, intentions, and emotions of others (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Adults with AS have difficulty understanding the intentions (cognitive ToM) and emotional impact of others' actions (affective ToM) as assessed by the Faux Pas Test (FPT) (Zalla et al., 2009). However, reports of adults with AS with the reading the mind in the eyes test (RMET) have shown impaired (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997, 2001) and preserved performance (Roeyers et al., 2001; Ponnet et al., 2004; Spek et al., 2010). These controversial results have been explained by the features of the RMET since correlations between RMET and other ToM measures are weak (Luzzatti et al., 2002; Spek et al., 2010).

Impairments in empathy, the capacity to share and understand the emotional states of others in reference to oneself (Decety and Moriguchi, 2007), are also a feature of the AS. Nevertheless, few studies have examined empathy in adults with AS. The majority of the studies (Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Rogers et al., 2007) have focused on self-report questionnaires. However, other reports (Dziobek et al., 2008) have represented an experimental assessment of empathy in adults with AS. These studies show that these patients are impaired in cognitive empathy but do not differ from controls in emotional empathy.

Finally, one study recently reported that participants with AS and HFA participants exhibit specific impairments in moral judgment. Participants made atypical moral judgments when they needed to consider the intention of harm (accidental vs. intentional) and the outcome (neutral vs. negative) of a person's actions (Moran et al., 2011). These participants were unable to judge the moral difference between accidental and attempted harms.

Relevant factors in AS social cognition research

As we mentioned above, to establish the underlying factors that contribute to the social cognition deficits of adults with AS, it is essential to consider several factors. First, to explore the social cognitive deficits in adults with AS, it is important to examine multiple domains with different tasks. Implicit social cognition tasks would require the spontaneous perception of the relevant contextual elements of the situation (Klin, 2000). Conversely, in explicit social cognition tasks the elements of the situation are clearly defined and these can usually be solved with relatively abstract and universal rules learned by explicit knowledge. Individuals with AS fail when they need to spontaneously apply social reasoning abilities to solve more naturalistic tasks, but when explicit information is provided, they improve the performance (Klin, 2000; Senju et al., 2009; Izuma et al., 2011). Thus, to assess several social cognition domains with different contextual clues involvement allows for a more comprehensive evaluation, and it makes it possible to establish whether there is a common factor that explains the adults with AS social cognition deficits. However, until now, only a few studies have simultaneously tested more than one social cognition domain.

Furthermore, most of previous social cognition reports (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Moran et al., 2011; Zalla et al., 2011) have included subjects diagnosed with AS and patients with other autism spectrum disorders (e.g., HFA). Therefore, the findings of these investigations can be biased by the sample selection. There is an ongoing debate about the differentiation among autistic subtypes, especially between AS and HFA. According to the DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) for autism, not for AS, delay in language and qualitative impairments in communication must be evident. However, several studies suggest that there is not only a difference in language abilities among HFA and AS (for a review see Matson and Wilkins, 2008). Unlike HFA, individuals with AS do not have delay in early cognitive functioning (Frith, 2004). Furthermore, AS compared to HFA individuals have more accentuated visual-motor deficits (Klin et al., 1995; Noterdaeme et al., 2010), less strong impairments in verbal comprehension (Noterdaeme et al., 2010; Planche and Lemonnier, 2012), higher verbal than performance IQ (Klin et al., 1995) and less severe behavioral abnormalities (Gilchrist et al., 2001). These evidences suggest that both of these disorders should be studied as separate diagnostic entities (Matson and Wilkins, 2008).

On the other hand, EF are required for the processing of emotional stimuli and social cognition tasks (Pessoa, 2008; Uekermann et al., 2010). Emotional processing requires holding stimuli in the working memory, and irrelevant information needs to be inhibited. In the same vein, ToM and empathy entail holding information in the working memory and switching between one's own perspective and that of another person (Uekermann et al., 2010). Nevertheless, no studies on adults with AS have controlled for the effect of EF on social cognition performance.

Finally, adults with AS perform variably among multiple domains (Hill and Bird, 2006; Towgood et al., 2009). This variability is observed more frequently in EF but also in social cognition. Deficits in working memory, cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control have been reported (Morris et al., 1999; Ambery et al., 2006; Hill and Bird, 2006), while other studies in adults with AS (Just et al., 2007; Nyden et al., 2010) have found preserved executive functioning. Affected (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997; Zalla et al., 2009) and intact performances (Ponnet et al., 2004; Spek et al., 2011) on ToM tasks have also been reported. These mixed findings suggest that patterns of deficits vary from individual to individual and that the adults with AS population include patients with both sub-normal and supra-normal performance. Thus, AS is more likely to be associated with a complex pattern of deficits across and within domains rather than just a single primary processing deficit (Happe et al., 2006). The heterogeneity in AS individuals has been interpreted as an obstacle to research (Happe et al., 2006). Traditional group-study type of analysis is problematic for individuals with high variability in performance because of the averaging artifact (Shallice and Evans, 1978).

The goal of this study

The primary goal of this study was to examine the performance of adults with AS on multiple social cognition domains with different levels of contextual integration while assessing the influence of EF. The social cognition domains evaluated were emotion recognition, ToM, empathy, moral judgment, social norms knowledge, and self-monitoring behavior in social settings. We included some tasks that require the implicit perception and integration of the relevant social elements to solve a social situation, and other in which the elements of the situation are explicitly defined and can be solved with relatively abstract and universal learned rules. In adittion, we explored the individual variability in the AS group. For this purpose, we employed a methodology called multiple case series analysis (MCSA) (Hill and Bird, 2006; Towgood et al., 2009), that detects the domains in which a given individual displays an abnormal performance. Group comparison analyses requires homogeneity between subjects; however, individuals with AS exhibit performance variability, which is concealed in these analyses. Therefore, the lack of significant differences is not necessarily an index of intact performance in this population (Hill and Bird, 2006).

Taking previous findings into account, we predicted that adults with AS will have deficits in several social cognition domains. We hypothesized that the social cognition deficits of adults with AS would be more related to impairments in the capacity to implicitly integrate action intentions with contextual clues than to the inability to apply explicit social rules. We also hypothesized that the social cognition difficulties would not be explained by EF profiles. This hypothesis was based on the fact that deficits in social cognition seem to be a fundamental characteristic that is less affected by AS heterogeneity, while patterns of EF have shown high variability between individuals. Finally, we predicted that the MCSA should demonstrate that patterns of cognitive strengths and weaknesses vary within individuals.

Materials and methods

Participants

Fifteen adult's diagnosed with AS and 15 healthy subjects participated in the present study. All participants were selected from the outpatient population of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology. All adults with AS had an estimated IQ above 94 (SD ≤ 7.42). Patients were assessed by a psychiatrist and met the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV) criteria for AS (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The diagnosis was made on the basis of the adult Asperger assessment (AAA) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005). Before the clinical interview, patients are asked to complete autism spectrum quotient (AQ) and the empathy quotient (EQ) as screening questionnaires (see Table Table2).2). The psychiatrist then sought to validate the symptom examples provided by the AQ and EQ and checked the other AS symptoms and criteria.

Table 2
Demographic and executive functions assessment.

Healthy control participants matched with the adults with AS were recruited from a large pool of volunteers. No significant differences in age [F(1, 28) = 0.003, p = 0.95], gender [X2(1) = 0.012, p = 0.91], handedness [X2(1) = 0.00, p = 1.00] or years of formal education [F(1, 28) = 1.36, p = 0.25] were observed between adults with AS and controls.

The following exclusion criteria were applied: (1) AS participants who met DSM-IV criteria for any axis-I diagnosis were excluded; (2) control subjects with a history of mental retardation, neurological disease, psychiatric disease, or any clinical condition that may affect cognitive performance were excluded; (3) adults with AS and controls with a history of drug or alcohol abuse were also excluded. All participants provided written informed consent in agreement with the Helsinki declaration. The study was approved by the ethics committee of Institute of Cognitive Neurology.

Materials and procedure

A battery of neuropsychological tests was used to assess EF and social cognition (see below). Patients were also evaluated with the Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). This scale includes vocabulary and matrix reasoning subtests and provides an estimated IQ (Weschler, 1999). All participants were individually evaluated in a quiet office of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology. A complete evaluation was administrated in one session that lasted approximately 2 h. Subjects were initially assessed with the social cognition tasks and then with the EF and intellectual level tests. The order of administration of the tasks was the same for each participant.

EF assessment

All participants were evaluated with an EF battery which included measures of verbal fluency, inhibitory control, interference control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. Verbal and design fluency tests (Delis and Kaplan, 2001) were used to assess recall, self-monitoring, and cognitive flexibility strategies. The trail-making test (Partington, 1949) was employed to assess cognitive flexibility and processing speed, and the Hayling test (Burgess and Shallice, 1996) was used to measure inhibitory control. The Flanker test (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) was applied to evaluate the ability to inhibit responses to irrelevant stimuli and the executive control of attention. The set shifting task (Diamond and Kirkham, 2005) was used to assess cognitive flexibility and inhibitory control. Finally, a span counting task (Case et al., 1982) and the 1-back test (Gevins and Cutillo, 1993) were applied to evaluate working memory.

Measures of social cognition

A description of social cognition tasks is provided in Table Table1.1. All participants were evaluated with a social cognition battery that included measures of emotion recognition, ToM, empathy, moral judgment, social norms knowledge, and self-monitoring behavior in social settings. The awareness of social inference test (TASIT) (McDonald et al., 2003, 2006; Kipps et al., 2009) was used to assess recognition of emotional states. This task introduces contextual cues (e.g., prosody, facial movement, and gestures) and additional processing demands (e.g., adequate speed of information processing, selective attention, and social reasoning) that are not taxed when viewing static displays. The RMET (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997) and the FPT (Stone et al., 1998) were applied to assess emotional and cognitive aspects of the ToM. An empathy for pain task (EPT; Couto et al., 2012) was employed to evaluate the empathy in the context of intentional and accidental harms. We also used the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI; Davis, 1983), a 28-item self-report questionnaire that measures both the cognitive and affective components of empathy. Finally, we included a moral judgment task (Young et al., 2010) and the revised self-monitoring scale (RSMS) (Lennox and Wolfe, 1984). A detailed description of the social cognition tasks is provided in supplementary data.

Table 1
Social cognition domain assessed and tasks employed.

Data analysis

The demographic, neuropsychological, and experimental data were compared between the groups using ANOVA and Tukey's HSD post-hoc test (when appropriate). The ANOVA results were also corrected for multiple comparisons using the Tukey's test. When analyzing categorical variables (e.g., gender), χ 2 square tests were applied. To control for the influence of EF on the performance on social cognition tasks, we applied an ANCOVA test that was adjusted for the cognitive flexibility score. The α value for all statistical tests was set at 0.05.

To assess individual differences, we conducted a MCSA and compared each participant with the control group on every performance measure. We followed the method of Towgood et al. (2009) and used a threshold of 2 standard deviations (SD) from the mean of the control group to define the normal range. First, we identified control subjects who displayed abnormal performance in each sub-measure, according to the 2 SD criteria, and removed them. Then, we recomputed the control means and SD excluding these subjects and identified adults with AS and control participants who were below (minus 2 SD) or above (plus 2 SD) the controls mean. We carried out frequency analyses in order to record the instances in which the performance of each subject was subnormal or supranormal. We then used non-parametric tests (Mann–Whitney tests) to compare the number of measures of impaired and supra-normal performance.

Finally, Pearson's correlations were performed to examine the association between the EF measures with the greatest variability, and the total scores on the social cognition tasks that were significantly different between groups.

Results

Table Table22 shows the overall results from the demographic and EF assessment.

Executive functions assessment

The results showed that our groups have similar EF performance. No differences in verbal fluency, inhibitory control, interference control, or working memory were observed (Table (Table2).2). However, the adults with AS performed significantly lower than controls on the switching design fluency task [F(1, 28) = 5.10, p < 0.05], suggesting subtle cognitive flexibility impairments. Given these results, we considered this measure as a covariate in the social cognition performance analysis.

Measures of social cognition

Figure Figure11 summarizes the significant differences between groups.

Figure 1
Significant differences between groups in social cognition tasks. (A) TASIT (accuracy per category). A, anger; D, disgust; SD, sadness; F, fear; SR, surprise. (B) Faux pas test (total score). (C) Scores on IRI subscales. EC, empathic concern; PD, personal ...

Recognition of emotional states

No significant differences in the TASIT total score were observed [F(1, 28) = 0.69, p = 0.41]. The per category analysis showed significant differences between groups [F(4, 108) = 7.97, p < 0.01]. A post-hoc analysis (Tukey HSD, MS = 0.49, df = 134.13) revealed that adults with AS had difficulty with disgust categorization (p < 0.01). This effect was preserved (p < 0.01) after co-varying for cognitive flexibility (p = 0.35). No significant differences were observed for anger (p = 1), fear (p = 0.22), sadness (p = 0.11) or surprise (p = 0.74) categorization.

Theory of mind

For the ToM measures, the adults with AS scored significantly lower than controls on the FPT total score [F(1, 28) = 20.62, p < 0.01]. This result did not change (p < 0.01) after adjusting for cognitive flexibility (p = 0.15). Significant differences were also observed on the hits [F(1, 28) = 20.62, p < 0.01]. Differences were preserved (p < 0.01) after co-varying for cognitive flexibility (p = 0.13). The AS group also showed lower intentionality scores [F(1, 28) = 74.21, p < 0.01]. This effect was preserved (p < 0.01) in the covariate analysis (p = 0.41). Furthermore, adults with AS scored lower on emotional attribution [F(1, 28) = 29.08, p < 0.01]. This effect was maintained (p < 0.01) after adjusting for the covariate (p = 0.43). No significant differences were observed on the reject scores [F(1, 28) = 0.007, p = 0.93].

No differences between the groups were observed on the RMET [F(1, 28) = 0.09, p = 0.76].

Empathy

Empathy for pain task. The ratings of empathic concern were significantly different between groups [F(2, 52) = 6.70, p < 0.01]. A post-hoc analysis (tukey hsd, ms = 10.62, df = 55.08) revealed that the adults with as rated the intentional pain situations with lower scores (p < 0.01), even controlling for cognitive flexibility (p = 0.65). Furthermore, the controls rated greater empathic concern for intentional harm situations than accidental harm situations (p < 0.01). However, this difference was not observed in the adults with as. Moreover, significant group differences were observed in the punishment ratings [F(2, 52) = 7.02, p < 0.01]. The post-hoc comparisons (tukey hsd, ms = 6.87, df = 66.7) showed that the adults with as tended to rate intentional harm situations with lower scores than controls (p = 0.06). This tendency did not change (p = 0.06) in the covariate analysis (p = 0.93). No differences were observed in the judgments of discomfort, intention to harm or correctness.

In addition, the RTs of the discomfort judgments were different between groups [F(2, 52) = 4.72, p < 0.05]. The RTs of the discomfort judgments were longer for the intentional harm than the neutral (p < 0.01) and accidental (p < 0.05) harm situations. These differences were preserved (p < 0.05) in the covariate analysis (p = 0.17).

IRI. Adults with as scored higher on pd subscale [F(1, 28) = 6.02, p < 0.05] than controls. This effect was preserved (p < 0.05) after adjusting for the covariate (p = 0.60). No difference between groups [F(1, 28) =1.96, p = 0.17] were observed on the ec subscale. Furthermore, the as group tended to have lower scores than controls [F(1, 28) =4.01, p = 0.055] on the pt subscale. This tendency was true (p < 0.01) after controlling for cognitive flexibility (p = 0.09). No difference between the groups was observed [F(1, 28) = 0.17, p = 0.67] on the F-subscale.

Moral judgment

In both groups, actions with neutral intentions [F(1, 28) = 146.29, p < 0.01] and neutral outcomes [F(1, 28) = 24.55, p < 0.01] were judged to be more permissible than actions with negative intentions and negative outcomes. Accidental harm was judged as being more permissible than intentional harm (Intention × Outcome Interaction) [F(1, 28) = 7.40, p < 0.01]. The group × intention × outcome interaction [F(1, 28) = 1.60, p = 0.21] was not statistically significant. Therefore, the adults with AS and controls did not differ in their judgments of morality. Specifically, the judgments of the neutral (neutral outcome, neutral intent), attempted harm (neutral outcome, harmful intent), accidental harm (harmful outcome, neutral intent), or intentional harm (harmful outcome, harmful intent) vignettes did not differ between groups.

Knowledge of social norms

No differences between groups were observed in the break [F(1, 24) = 0.50, p = 0.48] and over-adhere [F(1, 28) = 0.00, p = 1.00] scores of the SNQ.

Self-monitoring behavior in social settings

Adults with AS obtained lower scores in the sensitivity for expression behavior of others compared to controls [F(1, 28) = 29.26 p < 0.01], even after the covariate (p = 0.65). Adults with AS also received lower scores on the ability to modify self-presentation [F(1, 28) = 19.40, p < 0.01]. This effect remained true after the covariate analysis (p < 0.01), even though a significant effect of cognitive flexibility (p < 0.05) on self-presentation was observed.

In summary, adults with AS showed impairments on measures of disgust recognition (TASIT), ToM (FPT), and empathic concern and punishment ratings for the intentional harm situations (EPT). Additionally, the adults with AS showed higher scores on the PD subscale. They also showed lower scores on subscales of the sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others and the ability to modify self-presentation (RSMS). All differences were preserved after covarying for cognitive flexibility. Overall, adults with AS seem to perform less well in tasks that require an implicit encoding of socially relevant information and automatic context integration. Nevertheless, they performed as well as controls in tasks in which the social information was explicitly presented and when the task could be solved with abstract rules. Finally, the difficulties experienced by the adults with AS were not explained by abnormalities in EF.

Multiple case series analysis (MCSA)

To explore the intra-individual variability in tasks performance of the AS group, we examined the ranges of z-scores based on the performance of the control group (Towgood et al., 2009). The maximum range of performance on each of the 78 measures in controls was 4.60. Among the adults with AS, more than 43% of the measures (34/78 sub-measures) showed a z-score range exceeding the maximum threshold observed in controls. Specifically, 27.78% (5/18) of the EF measures exceeded the maximum range of the control group, whereas 48.33% (29/60) of the social cognition measures exceeded this range.

A greater number of adults with AS performed atypically compared with the control group. The individual performance profiles of each AS and control participants are provided in Appendix (see Tables A1a, A1b, A2a and A2b). The measures that were the most variable are detailed in Table Table3.3. Most of the adults with AS performed below normal (<2SD below control group mean) in both, EF and social cognition measures. With regard to EF, supra-normal (>2SD above control group mean) performance was observed only in the phonological fluency task. They also obtained supra-normal performance on several EPT measures. More specifically, the adults with AS showed supra-normal ratings in tasks involving neutral situations (e.g., discomfort, intention to hurt, and happiness ratings). In neutral scenarios in which the actions do not involve the intention to hurting someone, one would expect lower discomfort or intention to hurt ratings. Thus, the results suggest that the adults with AS are unable to discriminate between the neutral, accidental and intentional pain situations.

Table 3
The measures of executive functions and social cognition reveal variable performance in the AS group.

Consistent with the group analysis, the MCSA revealed that the adults with AS performed less well than the controls. Inter-individual variability (subnormal performance) was observed on: FPT (60%), TASIT (26.67%), empathic concern rating of intentional pain (33.33%), PD (33.33%), sensitivity of expression behavior of others (33.33%) and ability to modify self-presentation (53.33%).

To explore the inter-individual variability, we analyzed the performance of each participant and recorded instances in which the performance was 2 SDs below or above of the control mean. A non-parametric test was applied to compare the number of measures for subnormal and supra-normal performance (see Table Table4).4). As expected, the adults with AS showed a greater number of abnormal measures than controls (Mann–Whitney U = 19.00, p < 0.01). The AS participants also showed a greater number of measures in which they performed below control performance (Mann–Whitney U = 14.00, p < 0.01). However, no significant differences were observed in the number of measures with supra-normal performance (Mann–Whitney U = 82.00, p = 0.21).

Table 4
Comparison of the number of measures in which each individual exhibited abnormal performance.

In summary, the MCSA showed higher variability in the performance of the adults with AS compared with controls. A larger proportion of the social cognition measures compared to the EF measures exceeded the maximum range of the z-scores calculated based on the control group performance. In the AS group subnormal performance was higher than supra-normal.

Association between EF and social cognition performance

Finally, we explored the influence of EF on social cognition performance. We examined the correlation between the EF measures with the greatest variability, and the total scores on the social cognition tasks that were significantly different between groups. No significant correlations were observed.

Discussion

The primary goal of this study was to examine the performance of adults with AS on tasks of multiple domains of social cognition, while assessing the influence of EF. The secondary goal was to explore individual variability in adults with AS performance on both the social cognition and EF tasks. Our results suggest that participants with AS have a fundamental deficit in several domains of social cognition. We also found that the AS participants showed a greater number of social cognition measures in which they performed below controls' performance. These deficits were not explained by abnormalities in EF.

Furthermore, our data suggest that a common mechanism underlies the deficits in multiple social cognition domains in the adults with AS. In brief, these participants performed poorly on tasks (TASIT, FPT, EPT) that imply the ability to implicitly infer the intentionality of actions and those that require the integration of mental states (intentions, beliefs, emotions) with contextual information.

This is the first study in adults with AS to explore the effect of EF on social cognition performance. Both AS and control groups were similar regarding executive functioning. Moreover, to control for the effect of EF on performance during social cognition tasks, we conducted covariance analysis adjusted for cognitive flexibility, the only domain in which we found group significant differences. All significant differences in the social cognition measures remained significant. Moreover, we did not find significant correlations between scores on the EF measures with higher variability and those of the social cognition tasks that were different between groups. Because we selected tasks that were designed to assess specifically EF and have been utilized extensively to assess these domains (Partington, 1949; Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974; Case et al., 1982; Gevins and Cutillo, 1993; Burgess and Shallice, 1996; Delis and Kaplan, 2001; Diamond and Kirkham, 2005), we consider that the failure to find significant correlations could not be explained by the lack of the sensitivity of the executive measures. Instead, the lack of significant correlations may be explained by the low variability observed in the EF performance, since both groups had a similar executive functioning and low variability. Consequently, these results indicate that EF do not seem to play a major role in the social cognition impairments of adults with AS.

Deficits in social cognition

We employed an ecological task of contextual inference of emotional states (TASIT) which requires the integration of cues from face, prosody, gesture, and social context to identify the emotions. Consistent with previous reports (Ashwin et al., 2007; Falkmer et al., 2011), our results showed that individuals with AS have difficulty recognizing expressions of disgust. It has been shown that the basal ganglia, in parallel with the insula, are involved in disgust recognition (Calder et al., 2000; Adolphs, 2002; Wang et al., 2003; Ibáñez et al., 2010a,b). Fronto-insular networks seem to be crucial for social cognition (Couto et al., 2012). Individuals with AS show reduced gray matter in the basal ganglia (McAlonan et al., 2002; Nayate et al., 2005). They also show abnormalities in the white matter between the basal ganglia and thalamus, which connects brain areas (amygdala and fusiform gyrus) (McAlonan et al., 2009). Moreover, adults with AS present smaller volumes in the insular cortex (Kosaka et al., 2010). Therefore, the deficits in disgust recognition may be associated with abnormalities in the basal ganglia and the insula.

As previously reported (Ponnet et al., 2004; Spek et al., 2011), no differences between AS individuals and controls were found in ToM as measured by the RMET. Nevertheless, our data showed that the adults with AS performed poorly on the FPT, which is consistent with other studies (Zalla et al., 2009; Spek et al., 2011). In this test, adults with AS failed to identify the faux pas and to understand them as unintentional actions. Furthermore, they had difficulties to understand the emotional impact generated by the faux pas. The discrepancy in the performance between both ToM tests in the AS group can be explained by the features of these tasks. First, the FPT presents social scenarios resembling daily life situations. These tasks that involve real-life social scenarios are more sensitive to detect the ToM deficits of individuals with autism and AS (Klin, 2000). Furthermore, an adequate performance in the FPT involves the capacity to implicitly integrate cognitive inferences about mental states with empathic understanding. This capacity is mediated by the appraisal of contextual clues and relevant social elements provided in the scene information. Conversely, the RMET can be solved using basic and general matching strategies to correctly pair the depicted eyes and emotions. Thus, taken together, the ToM results suggest that adults with AS have difficulty integrating implicit information from the context and using this information to infer the intentionality and the emotional impact of the others' actions.

We employed a more ecologically valid measure of empathy (EPT) than the self-report questionnaires. In this task, the adults with AS showed abnormal empathic concern ratings, punishment ratings, and RTs of discomfort judgments for the intentional pain situations. Consistent with previous findings (Klin, 2000; Zalla et al., 2009), our results indicate that these individuals have difficulty with inferring the intentionality of actions. Information about intentionality allows us to decide how bad or good an action is. The deficit in intention inference may have affected the empathic concern ratings and therefore, the punishment ratings of the adults with AS.

In addition, the adults with AS showed higher levels of PD and a trend toward lower levels of PT compared with controls on the IRI. These results are supported by previous studies (Rogers et al., 2007; Dziobek et al., 2008). The high PD scores indicate greater levels of discomfort in interpersonal settings. This finding may be related to the slower RTs in the AS group for discomfort judgments in the intentional pain situations. Furthermore, individuals with AS show higher levels of anxiety (Hurtig et al., 2009; Lai et al., 2011), which may increase their PD scores. The lower scores on the PT subscale suggest that individuals with AS have difficulty understanding the feelings and perspectives of others, which is congruent with the EPT results.

In summary, the pattern of performance on the empathy measures indicated that adults with AS are impaired when using contextual information to infer the intentions of others. These deficits are reflected by lower ratings of empathic concern and punishment. Moreover, these individuals show higher levels of discomfort in stressful interpersonal situations.

Interestingly, we found that adults with AS performed similarly than control participants on measures of moral judgment. Both groups judged accidental harm as being more permissible than intentional harm. The lack of difference between groups in this task may be due to the fact that information about intention, outcome, and context (scene information) were presented in an explicit way. Therefore, it was possible to understand the moral content using two abstract rules with a linear relationship. For example, if the protagonist had the intention of harming another person (negative intent) and in fact caused harm (negative outcome); then the protagonist's action should be morally forbidden. Our results are in line with previous studies in individuals with AS (Klin, 2000; Izuma et al., 2011) that have shown intact performance or subtle deficits on tasks where explicit information is available. However, a recent study (Moran et al., 2011) employing a similar paradigm reported atypical moral judgment in individuals with AS and HFA. The discrepancy between these results and the current findings may be explained by the sample selection criteria employed in each study. Moran and colleagues included both HFA and AS participants. Individuals with HFA have language delay and usually present impairments in verbal skills (Baron-Cohen et al., 2005; Matson and Wilkins, 2008). These difficulties can affect their performance on the task. Thus, moral judgment in adults with AS needs to be further studied using naturalistic social situations without explicit rules.

On the other hand, this is the first attempt to investigate self-monitoring in social settings in an AS population. As expected, AS participants were less sensitive to the expressive behavior of other individuals, indicating that they had a low capacity for detecting implicit social and interpersonal cues. They also showed a diminished ability to modify self-presentation in social situations, suggesting that they had difficulty with adjusting their behaviors and with navigating novel or challenging social situations. Consistent with this idea, a negative correlation between self-monitoring and measures of social skills has been reported (Furnham and Capon, 1983). Furthermore, the ability to modify self-presentation is negatively correlated with social anxiety (Cramer and Gruman, 2002). Thus, the deficits in self-monitoring in social settings may be related to the lack of social skills and the high levels of anxiety (Hurtig et al., 2009; Lai et al., 2011) experienced by individuals with AS.

Moreover, our results revealed no differences between the AS participants and controls on the SNQ. This finding indicates that social rules knowledge is preserved in adults with AS. In accordance with our data, a study (Zalla et al., 2011) reported that AS and high-functioning individuals with autism are able to detect social rule violations. Furthermore, social norms can be learned in an explicit way. This explicit knowledge can be used by adults with AS to guide their behavior and can act as a compensatory strategy for their social cognition deficits.

Overall, consistent with our hypothesis, the adults with AS showed impairments in several social cognition domains (emotion recognition, ToM, empathy, and self-monitoring in social settings). Specifically, the adults with AS performed poorly on those social cognition tasks (TASIT, FPT, and EPT) that involve an implicit encoding of socially relevant information and the automatic integration of contextual information to solve a given social situation. Conversely, these individuals performed as well as controls in some tasks (RMET, moral judgment task, and SNQ) that had common features. In these tasks the elements of the situation are clearly defined and usually can be solved with relatively abstract and universal rules. This pattern of social cognition performance suggests that one underlying factor may explain the deficits. According to a recently proposed social context network model (Ibañez and Manes, 2012), this factor seems to be the implicit encoding and the integration of contextual information in order to access to the social meaning.

In addition, our results suggest that adults with AS may benefit from the use of explicit information. However, in most real-life situations, the social demands are not explicitly formulated. Social situations involve implicitly inferring the meaning of the circumstance by integrating contextual cues. Therefore, the pattern of deficits presented here may partially explain the difficulties with social interaction that individuals with AS experience in their daily lives.

Adults with AS may use abstract rules to compensate for their impairments in social cognition. Previous reports have shown that individuals with AS have superior abstract reasoning abilities (Hayashi et al., 2008; Soulieres et al., 2011). This strength may contribute to the performance on social cognition tasks that require the use of abstract rules and the integration of explicit information. On the other hand, this superiority in abstract reasoning may not help in social situations that involve implicit social rules and the integration of contextual cues. In these situations, the meaning of social information is less predictable and relies heavily on context, which reduces the chances of inferring the meaning by applying explicit abstract rules.

Variability in the performance of adults with AS

Adults with AS showed heterogeneous performance on several EF and social cognition tasks. These participants obtained mainly subnormal performance among the measures with the largest variability. Furthermore, this intra-individual variability was higher for the performances of social cognition than for the EF tests. The decreased variability of the EF tasks can be explained by the intact or superior fluid intelligence in adults with AS (Hayashi et al., 2008; Soulieres et al., 2011). Fluid intelligence is a major dimension of individual differences and refers to reasoning, abstract though and novel problem-solving ability (Duncan et al., 1995; Gray et al., 2003). Previous studies have suggested that high fluid intelligence is associated with better scores on EF tasks (Gray et al., 2003; Burgess and Braver, 2010) and indirectly related to psychosocial cognition (Huepe et al., 2011).

The current study is the first to explore the intra-individual variability of social cognition measures in adults with AS. Consistent with the group analysis, these patients obtained sub-normal performance on the same tasks (TASIT, FPT, EPT, IRI, and RSMS). Our data indicates that social cognition performance of adults with AS does not follow the same pattern of strengths and weaknesses reported in other cognitive domains (Hill and Bird, 2006; Towgood et al., 2009). Conversely, the social cognition patterns of individuals with AS is characterized by sub-normal performance, suggesting that these deficits are probably the core of the disorder.

Conclusions

Our study documents multiple social cognition deficits as fundamental features of the AS diagnosis. Our results showed that adults with AS present deficits in the implicit integration of contextual information in order to access to the social meaning. However, when social information is explicitly presented and the situation can be solved with abstract rules, the individuals with AS usually perform as well as controls. We also found that individual profiles of adults with AS showed subnormal performance in social cognition measures.

This is the first report in adults with AS to evaluate multiple social cognition domains assessing the EF and exploring inter- and intra-individual variability. However, some limitations of this study should be acknowledged. First, our sample size is relatively small, but it is similar to previous social cognition studies (Dziobek et al., 2008; Zalla et al., 2009; Moran et al., 2011) and it is also similar to other reports that have explored the cognitive variability of adults with AS (Hill and Bird, 2006; Towgood et al., 2009) and other patient populations (Deloche et al., 1999; Ramus et al., 2003). Moreover, unlike other reports (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004; Moran et al., 2011; Zalla et al., 2011), we only included individuals diagnosed with AS. Second, given the ongoing debate about the differentiation among autistic subtypes, especially between AS and HFA, future studies should compare social cognition profiles of both conditions. Further research should also explore the variability patterns of adults with AS compared with HFA. Third, although AS will probably be formally excluded as a diagnostic category in the DSM-V, our findings are still relevant for studying individual differences within autism spectrum disorders and the subset of people who show a particular profile (previously diagnosed as individuals with AS). In the future, detailed scientific assessments on cognitive domains, such as the ones presented in this work, may help to identify subcategories of autism spectrum disorders.

From a theoretical perspective, our findings are relevant for discussions on social cognition domain specificity in adults with AS. As previously proposed (Stone and Gerrans, 2006a,b), our results support a social cognition profile involving different degrees of affectation and a heterogeneous profile. These results do not support a modular or the “all or nothing” structure of social cognition. Contextual processing seems to affect the social cognition profile of adults with AS in a dissimilar way. For instance, their performance on social cognition tasks may be partially explained by the interaction of low-level mechanisms with the general capacity to integrate contextual information.

From a clinical perspective, our findings may have important implications for the diagnosis and treatment of the AS. The deficits found in multiple social cognition domains seem to be the core feature of the AS. It is also important to promote the use of tasks involving real-life social scenarios because these assessments are more sensitive to AS impairments (Klin, 2000). “Ecological” measures are context-sensitive tools that should be applied in neuropsychiatry (Burgess et al., 2009; Torralva et al., 2009; Ibañez and Manes, 2012).

In addition, the traditional social skills interventions for individuals with AS are based on learning explicit rules to build and foster relationships with others (Cappadocia and Weiss, 2011). However, the social skills acquired during those interventions do not generalize to situations outside of the treatment setting, which limits the efficacy of these programs (Rao et al., 2008; Cappadocia and Weiss, 2011). Thus, incorporating naturalistic environments into treatment may help individuals with AS generalize the learned social skills. Contextual integration of situated information seems to be crucial for several cognitive processes (Ibáñez et al., 2006, 2010a,b, 2011a,b, 2012; Hurtado et al., 2009; Aravena et al., 2010; Riveros et al., 2010; Amoruso et al., 2011, 2012; Barutta et al., 2011; Couto et al., 2012; Ibañez and Manes, 2012). Although implementation would be challenging, intervention programs should be based on teaching implicit rules for interpreting unpredictable social contexts. Learning to assess implicit contextual clues may improve the social skills of adults with AS.

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Ralph Adolphs and Phil Baker for their helpful and insightful comments in an earlier version of the paper. This research was partially supported by CONICET, FONDECYT (1130920) and INECO Foundation Grants. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of those grants.

Appendix

Measures of social cognition

Recognition of emotional states

The awareness of social inference test (TASIT). The TASIT is a test of social perception that involves videotaped vignettes of everyday social interactions (Kipps et al., 2009; McDonald et al., 2003, 2006). This task introduces contextual cues (e.g., prosody, facial movement, and gestures) and additional processing demands (e.g., adequate speed of information processing, selective attention, and social reasoning) that are not taxed when viewing static displays. We only considered part 1, called the emotion evaluation test (EET), which assesses recognition of emotional expression (fearful, surprised, sad, angry, and disgusted). In the EET, speaker demeanor (voice, facial expression, and gesture) together with the social situation indicate the emotional meaning. In some scenes, there is only one actor talking, who is either on the telephone or talking directly to the camera. Other scenes depict two actors and instructions are given to focus on one of them. All scripts are neutral in content and do not lend themselves to any particular emotion. The brief EET comprises a series of 20 short (15–60 s) videotaped vignettes of trained professional actors interacting in everyday situations. After viewing each scene, the test participant is instructed to choose from a forced-choice list the emotion expressed by the focused actor.

ToM

Reading the mind in the eyes (RMET)

This test (Baron-Cohen et al., 1997) assesses the emotional inference aspect of the ToM (or empathic accuracy). This is a computerized and validated test in which consist of 17 pictures of the eye region of a face. Participants are asked to choose which of four words best describes what the person in each photograph is thinking or feeling.

Faux pas test (FPT)

The FPT assesses the emotional and cognitive inference aspects of the ToM. In this task, the participants read stories that may contain a social faux pas (Stone et al., 1998). After each story was read, the subject is asked whether someone said something awkward (in order to identify stories containing a faux pas). Each story was presented in front of the patient in order to decrease working memory load. Performance was scored regarding the adequate identification of the faux pas (hits) and the adequate rejection of those stories which did not contain a faux pas (rejects). The score was 1 point for each faux pas correctly identified (maximum: 10), or non-faux pas correctly rejected (maximum: 10). A total score was computed (out of 20 total points) by adding the number of hits and rejects. When a faux pas was correctly identified, subjects were also asked 2 additional questions to measure intentionality—that is, recognizing that the person committing the faux pas was unaware that they had said something inappropriate (maximum 10)—and emotional attribution, in which participants should recognize that the person hearing the faux pas might have felt hurt or insulted (maximum 10).

Empathy

Empathy for pain task (EPT)

The EPT evaluates the empathy in the context of intentional and accidental harms. The task consists of 25 animated situations involving two individuals that are presented successively (Decety et al., 2011). The three following kinds of situations were depicted: intentional pain in which one person (passive performer) is in a painful situation caused intentionally by another (active performer), e.g., stepping purposely on someone's toe (pain caused by other); accidental pain where one person is in a painful situation accidentally caused by another; and control or neutral situations (e.g., one person receiving a flower given by another).

Importantly, the faces of the protagonists are not visible and there was no emotional reaction visible to the participants. We measured the ratings and reaction times (RTs) to situation comprehension (e.g., “press the button as soon as you understand the situation”). In addition, we assessed 7 questions about the following aspects of the scenarios: intentionality, e.g., the accidental or deliberate nature of the action; emphatic concern (how sad you feel for the victim); degree of discomfort (for the victim); harmful behavior (how bad was the purpose of the perpetrator); the valence behavior of the active perpetrator (how much positive emotion he/she felt in performing the action); the correctness of the action (moral judgment); and finally punishment (how much penalty this action deserves). Each question was answered using a computer-based visual analogue scale giving 7 different ratings by trial. Accuracy, RTs and ratings were measured.

Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (Davis, 1983). The IRI is a 28-item self-report questionnaire that separately measures both the cognitive and affective components of empathy. The instrument contains four scales: Perspective Taking (PT), Empathic Concern (EC), Fantasy (F), and Personal Distress (PD).

Moral judgment

Moral judgment task

Following the protocol reported elsewhere (Young et al., 2010), we presented participants with 24 scenarios. The four variations of each scenario followed a 2 × 2 design: (1) the protagonists either harmed another person (negative outcome) or did no harm (neutral outcome); (2) the protagonists either believed that they would cause harm (negative intent) or believed that they would cause no harm (neutral intent). Each possible belief was true for one outcome and false for the other outcome. The agent held true beliefs in the all-neutral and all-negative conditions and false beliefs in the accidental harm and attempted harm conditions. The participants saw one version of each scenario. In total, eight possible versions of the 24 scenarios with six trials of each of the four conditions were presented. The stimuli were presented in a pseudorandom order and the conditions were counterbalanced across participants. Each participant read six stories in each of the four conditions. After reading each story, the participants were asked to rate the scenario on a Likert-scale ranging from totally permissible (7) to totally forbidden (1).

Table A1a

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0001.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0001.jpg

Individual profiles of executive functions tasks performance for each adult with the AS.

Cells numbered with “1” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs above the control mean (subnormal performers). Cells numbered with “2” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs below the control mean (supra-normal performers).

Note. P. Fluency, Phonological Fluency; Simple F.D., Simple Fluency Design Task; Switching F.D., Switching Fluency Design Task; RT, reaction Time; ACC, accuracy.

Table A1b

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0002.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0002.jpg

Individual profiles of executive functions tasks performance for each control adult.

Cells numbered with “1” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs above the control mean (impaired performers). Cells numbered with “2” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs below the control mean (supra-normal performers).

Note. P. Fluency, Phonological Fluency; Simple F.D., Simple Fluency Design Task; Switching F.D., Switching Fluency Design Task; RT, reaction Time; Acc, accuracy.

Table A2a

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0003.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0003.jpg

Individual profiles of social cognition tasks performance for each adult with the AS.

Cells numbered with “1” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs above the control mean (subnormal performers). Cells numbered with “2” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs below the control mean (supra-normal performers).

Note. RTs = reaction times; SC = situation comprehension; EC = empathic concern; D=discomfort; IH=intention to hurt; C=correctness; P=punishment; I=intentionality.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0004.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0004.jpg

Note. TASIT, The Awareness of Social Inference Test; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; FPT, Faux Pas Test; PT, Perspective Taking; F, Fantasy; EC, Empathic concern; PD, personal distress; S, sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others; D, difficulty to modify self-presentation; A, ability to modify self-presentation; SNQ, social norms questionnaire; OS, over adhere score; B, break score; N, neutral vignettes; Acc. H, accidental harm; Att. H, attempted harm; IH, intentional harm.

Table A2b

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0005.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0005.jpg

Individual profiles of social cognition tasks performance for each control adult.

Cells numbered with “1” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs above the control mean (subnormal performers). Cells numbered with “2” depict subjects whose performance was 2 SDs below the control mean (supra-normal performers).

Note. RTs, reaction times; SC, situation comprehension; EC, empathic concern; D, discomfort; IH, intention to hurt; C, correctness; P, punishment; I, intentionality.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.  Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0006.jpg Object name is fnhum-06-00302-i0006.jpg

Note. TASIT, The Awareness of Social Inference Test; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; FPT, Faux Pas Test; PT, Perspective Taking; F, Fantasy; EC, Empathic concern; PD, personal distress; S, sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others; D, difficulty to modify self-presentation; A, ability to modify self-presentation; SNQ, social norms questionnaire; OS, over adhere score; B, break score; N, neutral vignettes; Acc. H, accidental harm; Att. H, attempted harm; IH, intentional harm.

Social norms knowledge

Social norms questionnaire (SNQ)

The SNQ questionnaire consisting of 20 yes–no questions was used (Rankin et al., 2009). The participants were asked to determine whether a behavior would be appropriate in the presence of an acquaintance (not a close friend or family member) according to the mainstream culture. Two scores were derived. The break score was defined as the total number of errors made in the direction of breaking a social norm, and the over-adhere score was defined as the total number of errors made in the direction of over adherence to a perceived social norm.

Self-monitoring behavior in social settings

Revised self-monitoring scale (RSMS)

The RSMS is a 13-item instrument and assesses the tendency to regulate one's behavior to present a particular self in a social context (Lennox and Wolfe, 1984). The scale involves two styles of self-monitoring behavior: the ability to modify self-presentation (e.g., “in social situations, I have the ability to alter my behavior if I feel that something else is called for”) and the sensitivity to the expressive behavior of others (e.g., “I am often able to read people's true emotions correctly through their eyes”). The participants responded using a 6-point Likert-scale. The ratings ranged from 0 = “strongly disagree” to 5 = “strongly agree.”

References

  • Baron-Cohen S., Jollife T., Mortimore C., Robertson M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 34, 163–175. [PubMed]
  • Davis M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126.
  • Decety J., Michalska K. J., Kinzler K. D. (2011). The contribution of emotion and cognition to moral sensitivity: a neurodevelopmental study. Cereb. Cortex 22, 209–220. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr111. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Kipps C. M., Nestor P. J., Acosta-Cabronero J., Arnold R., Hodges J. R. (2009). Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: the role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain 132, 592–603. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn314. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lennox R. D., Wolfe R. N. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoring scale. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 1349–1364. [PubMed]
  • McDonald S., Bornhofen C., Shum D., Long E., Saunders C., Neulinger K. (2006). Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil. Rehabil. 28, 1529–1542. doi: 10.1080/09638280600646185. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • McDonald S., Flanagan S., Rollins J., Kinch J. (2003). TASIT: a new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 18, 219–238. [PubMed]
  • Rankin K. P., Salazar A., Gorno-Tempini M. L., Sollberger M., Wilson S. M., Pavlic D., et al. (2009). Detecting sarcasm from paralinguistic cues: anatomic and cognitive correlates in neurodegenerative disease. Neuroimage 47, 2005–2015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.077. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Stone V. E., Baron-Cohen S., Knight R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640–656. [PubMed]
  • Young L., Bechara A., Tranel D., Damasio H., Hauser M., Damasio A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron 65, 845–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.003. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]

References

  • Adolphs R. (1999). Social cognition and the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 469–479. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01399-6. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Adolphs R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 169–177. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00301-X. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ambery F. Z., Russell A. J., Perry K., Morris R., Murphy D. G. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism 10, 551–564. doi: 10.1177/1362361306068507. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • American Psychiatric Association. (ed.). (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
  • Amoruso L., Cardona J., Melloni M., Sede O. L., Ibáñez A. (2012). Contextual impairments in schizophrenia and the FN400. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:191. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00191. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Amoruso L., Couto J. B., Ibáñez A. (2011). Beyond extrastriate body area (EBA) and fusiform body area (FBA): context integration in the meaning of actions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:124. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00124. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Aravena P., Hurtado E., Riveros R., Cardona J. F., Manes F., Ibáñez A. (2010). Applauding with closed hands: neural signature of action-sentence compatibility effects. PLoS ONE 5:e11751. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011751. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ashwin C., Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., O'Riordan M., Bullmore E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia 45, 2–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.014. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ashwin C., Chapman E., Colle L., Baron-Cohen S. (2006). Impaired recognition of negative basic emotions in autism: a test of the amygdala theory. Soc. Neurosci. 1, 349–363. doi: 10.1080/17470910601040772. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Atkinson A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia 47, 3023–3029. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.05.019. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Baron-Cohen S., Jollife T., Mortimore C., Robertson M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 34, 163–175. [PubMed]
  • Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A. M., Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. (2004). The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 34, 163–175. [PubMed]
  • Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J. J., Raste Y., Plumb I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251. [PubMed]
  • Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Robinson J., Woodbury-Smith M. (2005). The Adult Asperger Assessment (AAA): a diagnostic method. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35, 807–819. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0026-5. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Barutta J., Cornejo C., Ibáñez A. (2011). Theories and theorizers: a contextual approach to theories of cognition. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 45, 223–246. doi: 10.1007/s12124-011-9156-9. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Burgess G. C., Braver T. S. (2010). Neural mechanisms of interference control in working memory: effects of interference expectancy and fluid intelligence. PLoS ONE 5:e12861. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012861. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Burgess P. W., Alderman N., Volle E., Benoit R. G., Gilbert S. J. (2009). Mesulam's frontal lobe mystery re-examined. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 27, 493–506. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2009-0511. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Burgess P. W., Shallice T. (1996). Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 34, 263–272. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00104-2. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Calder A. J., Keane J., Manes F., Antoun N., Young A. W. (2000). Impaired recognition and experience of disgust following brain injury. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1077–1078. doi: 10.1038/80586. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Cappadocia M. C., Weiss J. A. (2011). Review of social skills training groups for youth with asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 5, 70–78. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0343-3. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Case R., Kurland D., Goldberg J. (1982). Operational efficiency and the growth of short-term memory span. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 33, 386–404.
  • Couto B., Sedeño L., Sposato L. A., Sigman M., Riccio C. A., Salles A., et al. (2012). Insular networks for emotional processing and social cognition: comparison of two case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement. Cortex S0010–9452, 245–246. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.006. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Cramer K., Gruman J. (2002). The lennox and wolfe revised self-monitoring scale: latent structure and gender invariance. Pers. Individ. Dif. 32, 627–637.
  • Davis M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126.
  • Decety J., Moriguchi Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. Biopsychosoc. Med. 1, 22. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-22. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Delis D., Kaplan B. (2001). The Delis-Kaplan Executive Functions System. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Deloche G., Souza L., Willadino-Braga L., Dellatolas G. (1999). Assessment of calculation and number processing by adults: cognitive and neuropsychological issues. Percept. Mot. Skills 89, 707–738. [PubMed]
  • Diamond A., Kirkham N. (2005). Not quite as grown-up as we like to think: parallels between cognition in childhood and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 16, 291–297. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01530.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Duncan J., Burgess P., Emslie H. (1995). Fluid intelligence after frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia 33, 261–268. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)00124-8. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Dziobek I., Rogers K., Fleck S., Bahnemann M., Heekeren H. R., Wolf O. T., et al. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Eriksen B., Eriksen W. (1974). Effect of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychopsys. 16, 143–145.
  • Falkmer M., Bjällmark A., Larsson M., Falkmer T. (2011). Recognition of facially expressed emotions and visual search strategies in adults with Asperger syndrome. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 5, 210–217.
  • Fombonne E., Tidmarsh L. (2003). Epidemiologic data on Asperger disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 12, 15–21, v–vi. [PubMed]
  • Frith U. (2004). Emanuel Miller lecture: confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 672–686. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00262.x. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Furnham A., Capon M. (1983). Social skills and self-monitoring processes. Pers. Individ. Dif. 4, 171–178.
  • Gevins A., Cutillo B. (1993). Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 87, 128–143. [PubMed]
  • Gilchrist A., Green J., Cox A., Burton D., Rutter M., Le Couteur A. (2001). Development and current functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: a comparative study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 227–240. [PubMed]
  • Gray J. R., Chabris C. F., Braver T. S. (2003). Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 316–322. doi: 10.1038/nn1014. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Happe F., Ronald A., Plomin R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 1218–1220. doi: 10.1038/nn1770. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hayashi M., Kato M., Igarashi K., Kashima H. (2008). Superior fluid intelligence in children with Asperger's disorder. Brain Cogn. 66, 306–310. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2007.09.008. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hill E., Bird C. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia 44, 2822–2835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.007. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hill E. L., Bird C. M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia 44, 2822–2835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.007. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hobson R. P., Ouston J., Lee A. (1988). Emotion recognition in autism: coordinating faces and voices. Psychol. Med. 18, 911–923. [PubMed]
  • Hubert B., Wicker B., Moore D. G., Monfardini E., Duverger H., Da Fonseca D., et al. (2007). Brief report: recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 1386–1392. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0275-y. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Huepe D., Roca M., Salas N., Canales-Johnson A., Rivera-Rei A. A., Zamorano L., et al. (2011). Fluid intelligence and psychosocial outcome: from logical problem solving to social adaptation. PLoS ONE 6:e24858. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024858. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hurtado E., Haye A., González R., Manes F., Ibáñez A. (2009). Contextual blending of ingroup/outgroup face stimuli and word valence: LPP modulation and convergence of measures. BMC Neurosci. 10:69. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-69. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Hurtig T., Kuusikko S., Mattila M. L., Haapsamo H., Ebeling H., Jussila K., et al. (2009). Multi-informant reports of psychiatric symptoms among high-functioning adolescents with Asperger syndrome or autism. Autism 13, 583–598. doi: 10.1177/1362361309335719. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., Cardona J. F., Dos Santos Y. V., Blenkmann A., Aravena P., Roca M., et al. (2012). Motor-language coupling: direct evidence from early Parkinson's disease and intracranial cortical recordings. Cortex. [Epub ahead of print]. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.02.014. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., Gleichgerrcht E., Hurtado E., Gonzalez R., Haye A., Manes F. F. (2010a). Early neural markers of implicit attitudes: N170 modulated by intergroup and evaluative contexts in IAT. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:188. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00188. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., Gleichgerrcht E., Manes F. (2010b). Clinical effects of insular damage in humans. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 397–410. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0256-y. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., LĂłpez V., Cornejo C. (2006). ERPs and contextual semantic discrimination: degrees of congruence in wakefulness and sleep. Brain Lang. 98, 264–275. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.05.005. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibañez A., Manes F. (2012). Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 78, 1354–1362. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182518375. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., Riveros R., Aravena P., Vergara V., Cardona J. F., Garcia L., et al. (2011a). When context is difficult to integrate: cortical measures of congruency in schizophrenics and healthy relatives from multiplex families. Schizophr. Res. 126, 303–305. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.04.008. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ibáñez A., Toro P., Cornejo C., Hurquina H., Manes F., Weisbrod M., et al. (2011b). High contextual sensitivity of metaphorical expressions and gesture blending: a video event-related potential design. Psychiatry Res. 191, 68–75. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.08.008. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Izuma K., Matsumoto K., Camerer C. F., Adolphs R. (2011). Insensitivity to social reputation in autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 17302–17307. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1107038108. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Just M. A., Cherkassky V. L., Keller T. A., Kana R. K., Minshew N. J. (2007). Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb. Cortex 17, 951–961. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl006. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Kipps C. M., Nestor P. J., Acosta-Cabronero J., Arnold R., Hodges J. R. (2009). Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: the role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain 132, 592–603. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn314. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Klin A. (2000). Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 831–846. [PubMed]
  • Klin A., Volkmar F. R., Sparrow S. S., Cicchetti D. V., Rourke B. P. (1995). Validity and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: convergence with nonverbal learning disabilities syndrome. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 36, 1127–1140. [PubMed]
  • Kosaka H., Omori M., Munesue T., Ishitobi M., Matsumura Y., Takahashi T., et al. (2010). Smaller insula and inferior frontal volumes in young adults with pervasive developmental disorders. Neuroimage 50, 1357–1363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.085. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lai M. C., Lombardo M. V., Pasco G., Ruigrok A. N., Wheelwright S. J., Sadek S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum conditions. PLoS ONE 6:e20835. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020835. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Lennox R. D., Wolfe R. N. (1984). Revision of the self-monitoring scale. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 1349–1364. [PubMed]
  • Lugnegard T., Hallerback M. U., Gillberg C. (2011). Psychiatric comorbidity in young adults with a clinical diagnosis of Asperger syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 1910–1917. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.03.025. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Luzzatti C., Raggi R., Zonca G., Pistarini C., Contardi A., Pinna G. D. (2002). Verb-noun double dissociation in aphasic lexical impairments: the role of word frequency and imageability. Brain Lang. 81, 432–444. doi: 10.1006/brln.2001.2536. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Macdonald H., Rutter M., Howlin P., Rios P., Le Conteur A., Evered C., et al. (1989). Recognition and expression of emotional cues by autistic and normal adults. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30, 865–877. [PubMed]
  • Matson J. L., Wilkins J. (2008). Nosology and diagnosis of Asperger's syndrome. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2, 288–300.
  • McAlonan G. M., Cheung C., Cheung V., Wong N., Suckling J., Chua S. E. (2009). Differential effects on white-matter systems in high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome. Psychol. Med. 39, 1885–1893. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709005728. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • McAlonan G. M., Daly E., Kumari V., Critchley H. D., Van Amelsvoort T., Suckling J., et al. (2002). Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome. Brain 125, 1594–1606. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf150. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • McDonald S., Bornhofen C., Shum D., Long E., Saunders C., Neulinger K. (2006). Reliability and validity of The Awareness of Social Inference Test (TASIT): a clinical test of social perception. Disabil. Rehabil. 28, 1529–1542. doi: 10.1080/09638280600646185. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • McDonald S., Flanagan S., Rollins J., Kinch J. (2003). TASIT: a new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 18, 219–238. [PubMed]
  • Moran J. M., Young L. L., Saxe R., Lee S. M., O'Young D., Mavros P. L., et al. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2688–2692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011734108. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Morris R. G., Rowe A., Fox N., Feigenbaum J. D., Miotto E. C., Howlin P. (1999). Spatial working memory in Asperger's syndrome and in patients with focal frontal and temporal lobe lesions. Brain Cogn. 41, 9–26. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1093. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Nayate A., Bradshaw J. L., Rinehart N. J. (2005). Autism and Asperger's disorder: are they movement disorders involving the cerebellum and/or basal ganglia? Brain Res. Bull. 67, 327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2005.07.011. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Noterdaeme M., Wriedt E., Hohne C. (2010). Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism: language, motor and cognitive profiles. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 19, 475–481. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0057-0. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Nyden A., Niklasson L., Stahlberg O., Anckarsater H., Wentz E., Rastam M., et al. (2010). Adults with autism spectrum disorders and ADHD neuropsychological aspects. Res. Dev. Disabil. 31, 1659–1668. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.04.010. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Partington J. (1949). Partington's pathway test. Psychol. Center Bull. 1, 9–20.
  • Pessoa L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 148–158. doi: 10.1038/nrn2317. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Philip R. C., Whalley H. C., Stanfield A. C., Sprengelmeyer R., Santos I. M., Young A. W., et al. (2010). Deficits in facial, body movement and vocal emotional processing in autism spectrum disorders. Psychol. Med. 40, 1919–1929. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992364. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Planche P., Lemonnier E. (2012). Children with high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome: can we differentiate their cognitive profiles? Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 6, 939–948. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1469-0. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ponnet K. S., Roeyers H., Buysse A., De Clercq A., Van Der Heyden E. (2004). Advanced mind-reading in adults with Asperger syndrome. Autism 8, 249–266. doi: 10.1177/1362361304045214. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Ramus F., Rosen S., Dakin S. C., Day B. L., Castellote J. M., White S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 126, 841–865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Rao P. A., Beidel D. C., Murray M. J. (2008). Social skills interventions for children with Asperger's syndrome or high-functioning autism: a review and recommendations. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 353–361. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0402-4. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Riveros R., Manes F., Hurtado E., Escobar M., Martin Reyes M., Cetkovich M., et al. (2010). Context-sensitive social cognition is impaired in schizophrenic patients and their healthy relatives. Schizophr. Res. 116, 297–298. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.10.017. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Roeyers H., Buysse A., Ponnet K., Pichal B. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 271–278. [PubMed]
  • Rogers K., Dziobek I., Hassenstab J., Wolf O. T., Convit A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 709–715. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Senju A., Southgate V., White S., Frith U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: an absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science 325, 883–885. doi: 10.1126/science.1176170. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Shallice T., Evans M. E. (1978). The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex 14, 294–303. [PubMed]
  • Soulieres I., Dawson M., Gernsbacher M. A., Mottron L. (2011). The level and nature of autistic intelligence II: what about Asperger syndrome? PLoS ONE 6:e25372. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025372. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Spek A. A., Scholte E. M., Van Berckelaer-Onnes I. A. (2010). Theory of mind in adults with HFA and Asperger syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 40, 280–289. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0860-y. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Spek A. A., Scholte E. M., Van Berckelaer-Onnes I. A. (2011). Local information processing in adults with high functioning autism and asperger syndrome: the usefulness of neuropsychological tests and self-reports. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 859–869. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1106-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Stone V. E., Baron-Cohen S., Knight R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 640–656. [PubMed]
  • Stone V. E., Gerrans P. (2006a). Does the normal brain have a theory of mind? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 3–4. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.010. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Stone V. E., Gerrans P. (2006b). What's domain-specific about theory of mind? Soc. Neurosci. 1, 309–319. doi: 10.1080/17470910601029221. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Torralva T., Roca M., Gleichgerrcht E., Bekinschtein T., Manes F. (2009). A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain 132, 1299–1309. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp041. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Towgood K. J., Meuwese J. D., Gilbert S. J., Turner M. S., Burgess P. W. (2009). Advantages of the multiple case series approach to the study of cognitive deficits in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 47, 2981–2988. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.028. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Uekermann J., Kraemer M., Abdel-Hamid M., Schimmelmann B. G., Hebebrand J., Daum I., et al. (2010). Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 734–743. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.009. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Wang K., Hoosain R., Yang R. M., Meng Y., Wang C. Q. (2003). Impairment of recognition of disgust in Chinese with Huntington's or Wilson's disease. Neuropsychologia 41, 527–537. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00171-9. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Weschler D. (1999). Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
  • Young L., Bechara A., Tranel D., Damasio H., Hauser M., Damasio A. (2010). Damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex impairs judgment of harmful intent. Neuron 65, 845–851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.003. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Zalla T., Barlassina L., Buon M., Leboyer M. (2011). Moral judgment in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Cognition 121, 115–126. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.004. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]
  • Zalla T., Sav A. M., Stopin A., Ahade S., Leboyer M. (2009). Faux pas detection and intentional action in Asperger Syndrome. a replication on a French sample. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 373–382. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0634-y. [PubMed] [Cross Ref]